Connect
MJA
MJA

Evidence-based physicians' dressing: a crossover trial

Balakrishnan R Nair, Stephen R Mears, Karen I Hitchcock and John R Attia
Med J Aust 2002; 177 (11): 681-682. || doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2002.tb05017.x
Published online: 9 December 2002

Abstract

Objective: To describe the effect of physicians' dress on patient confidence and trust.

Design: A prospective crossover trial involving physicians dressed in "respectable" versus "retro" attire.

Setting: A general medicine ward at a tertiary hospital.

Participants: 12 male general physicians and 1680 patients.

Main outcome measures: Patient trust and confidence as measured by a questionnaire mailed after hospital discharge.

Results: Formal attire was correlated with higher patient confidence and trust. Nose rings were particularly deleterious to patients' reported trust and confidence. A minimum threshold of two items of formal attire (dress pants, dress shirt, tie, or white coat) were necessary to inspire a reasonable amount of confidence; this is the NND (number needed to dress).

Conclusions: We highlight the need for more research into the effects of physician dress, and coin the term "evidence-based dressing".

In this era of evidence-based medicine (EBM),1 it is often bemoaned that the context in which healthcare is delivered is ignored or subsumed by a focus on the intervention. This context of healthcare delivery includes the patient–doctor relationship,2 its geographical and cultural setting, and the characteristics of an individual practitioner. There is, for example, no evidence-based answer to the question, "Does a doctor's appearance constitute an important and neglected aspect of the clinical context?".

Isaacs and Fitzgerald have argued that a lack of evidence for a treatment can be overcome with an "eloquence-based medicine" — one in which impeccable, brand-name-correct dress sense takes precedence over science.3 One can argue, as they do, that a professional appearance encourages our patients to trust us and have confidence in our abilities. Indeed, a study of parents of children admitted to a paediatric ward indicated they were twice as likely to attribute competence to physicians wearing formal attire.4 This begs the question of what constitutes "formal attire" — that is, what is the uniform of evidence-based dressing (EBD)?

In a study of 275 outpatients, a name badge, white coat, dress pants, dress shoes, traditional hair-style and skirt were all voted to be desirable attire for doctors. In the same study sandals, blue jeans, an open shirt and non-traditional hair-style were deemed undesirable.5

Traditionally, the physician's attire has included a white coat and tie,6,7 with the stethoscope used as an accessory.8 However, the evidence also suggests that these very icons that give physicians their credibility may be doing more harm than good. A white coat may prevent the tie from dangling in patients' wounds and secretions, but white coats have been shown to harbour potential contaminants.9,10 Ties themselves, dangling or not, have been shown to carry infection.11 Bow-ties have traditionally been expected to be associated with less contamination, but a multicentre randomised controlled trial showed that there was no sustained difference between necktie and bow-tie contamination rates.12

We are therefore faced with a dilemma: EBM dictates that ties are dangerous, but EBD dictates that we wear them.13 There is a marked lack of level 1 or level 2 evidence to support any clinical guidelines regarding doctors' dress. We therefore conducted a multiple crossover dressing trial, not to be confused with a cross-dressing trial, to analyse the effect of various items of clothing in inspiring patient confidence.

Discussion

Our results indicate strong contextual effects for the delivery of care, with physicians' dress influencing patient perceptions. Further work should define the effect of specific fashions on specific patient groups. We also believe that there is much potential in recognising fashion accessories as potential adjuvant therapies, and we advocate a new specialty called PCAM (physicians' complementary accessories medicine). By analogy to the NNT (number needed to treat), we calculate the NND (number needed to dress); this equates to any two of the four items — white coat, tie, dress pants, dress shirt — from the evidence-based wardrobe (EBW). We also take this opportunity to highlight a forthcoming joint venture between medical organisations and a department store in launching the Evidence-Based Wardrobe label. This label will have several lines, from the top-of-the-range "Level 1", for which there is the most evidence of therapeutic success, to the budget-based "Level 3". Medical organisations will also invite expressions of interest from those interested in operating EBC (Evidence-Based Cuts), a barber shop franchise to be located in teaching hospitals throughout Australia. Operators will need to adhere to strict quality-assurance guidelines, and demonstrate continuing competence through CBE (continuing barber education) to remain at the cutting edge of EBC and maintain their accreditation.

  • Balakrishnan R Nair1
  • Stephen R Mears2
  • Karen I Hitchcock3
  • John R Attia4

  • 1 John Hunter Hospital, New Lambton, NSW.
  • 2 Centre for Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Royal Newcastle Hospital, Newcastle, NSW.


Correspondence: knair@mail.newcastle.edu.au

Acknowledgements: 

The photograph accompanying this article was produced by Stephen McInally of Medical Communications, John Hunter Hospital.

Competing interests:

Professor Nair is a white-coated, bow-tied, greying physician who has received complimentary neckties, boxers and socks from pharmaceutical companies. Dr Attia is a general internist who has never been accused of being fashionable. Mr Mears is an open-shirted, jeans-clad informationist. All teach EBM. Karen Hitchcock is a final-year medical student. Some of her best friends have navel rings.

  • 1. Sackett DL, Rosenberg WM, Gray JA. Evidence based medicine: what it is and what it isn't. BMJ 1996; 312: 71-72.
  • 2. Di Blasi Z, Harkness E, Ernst E, et al. Influence of context effects on health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet 2001; 357: 757-762.
  • 3. Isaacs D, Fitzgerald D. Seven alternatives to evidence based medicine. BMJ 1999; 319: 1618.
  • 4. Taylor PG. Does dress influence how parents first perceive house staff competence? Am J Dis Child 1987; 141: 426-428.
  • 5. Kanzler MH, Gorsulowsky DC. Patients' attitudes regarding physical characteristics of medical care providers in dermatologic practices. Arch Dermatol 2002; 138: 463-466.
  • 6. Harnett PR. Should doctors wear white coats ? Med J Aust 2001; 174: 343-344. <eMJA full text>
  • 7. Druss RG. The magic white coat. Ann Intern Med 1998; 129: 743.
  • 8. Campbell JD. The stethoscope at ease. CMAJ 2001; 164: 748.
  • 9. Wong D, Nye K, Hollis P. Microbial flora on doctors' white coats. BMJ 1991; 303: 1602-1604.
  • 10. Varghese D, Patel H. Hand washing: stethoscopes and white coats are sources of nosocomial infection. BMJ 1999; 319: 519.
  • 11. Dixon M. Neck ties as vectors for nosocomial infection. Intensive Care Med 2000; 26: 250.
  • 12. Biljan MM, Hart CA, Sunderland D, et al. Multicentre randomised double blind crossover trial on contamination of conventional ties and bow ties in routine obstetric and gynaecological practice. BMJ 1993; 307: 1582-1584.
  • 13. McKinstry B, Wang JX. Putting on the style: what patients think of the way their doctor dresses. Br J Gen Pract 1991; 41: 275-278.
  • 14. Streiner D, Norman G; PDQ Epidemiology; Hamilton, Ontario: BC Decker, 1998.

Author

remove_circle_outline Delete Author
add_circle_outline Add Author

Comment
Do you have any competing interests to declare? *

I/we agree to assign copyright to the Medical Journal of Australia and agree to the Conditions of publication *
I/we agree to the Terms of use of the Medical Journal of Australia *
Email me when people comment on this article

Online responses are no longer available. Please refer to our instructions for authors page for more information.