Connect
MJA
MJA

An analysis of the causes of adverse events from the Quality in Australian Health Care Study

Bernadette T Harrison, Robert W Gibberd and John D Hamilton
Med J Aust 1999; 170 (9): 411-415.
Published online: 3 May 1999

Research

An analysis of the causes of adverse events from the
Quality in Australian Health Care Study

Ross McL Wilson, Bernadette T Harrison, Robert W Gibberd and John D Hamilton

MJA 1999; 170: 411-415
For editorial comment, see Vincent

Abstract - Introduction - Methods - Results - Discussion - Acknowledgements - References - Authors' details
- - More articles on Administration and health services


The Quality in Australian Health Care Study (QAHCS), published in the Journal in 1995,1 reported that 16.6% of hospital admissions were associated with an iatrogenic patient injury, termed an adverse event (AE) (see Box 1). This compares with the rate of 3.7% for AEs in the Harvard Medical Practice Study (HMPS),2 and a rate of 17% in a more recent study with an alternative observational method of determining AEs.3
Fifty per cent of the AEs in the QAHCS were judged to have a high preventability score (4 or more on a scale of 1-6 of increasing likelihood of preventability).1 The disability caused by these adverse events ranged from temporary disability (fully resolved in one month) in 46.6% of AEs, to death in 4.9% of AEs. Although recording AEs emphasises only the "complications" of rather than the benefits derived from healthcare, AEs are of great significance to individual patients as well as to the whole healthcare system.

Abstract Objective: To examine the causes of adverse events (AEs) resulting from healthcare to assist in developing strategies to minimise preventable patient injury.
Design: Descriptions of the 2353 AEs previously reported by the Quality in Australian Health Care Study (QAHCS) were reviewed. A qualitative approach was used to develop categories for human error and for prevention strategies to minimise these errors. These categories were then used to classify the AEs identified in the QAHCS, and the results were analysed with previously reported preventability and outcome data.
Results: 34.6% of the causes of AEs were categorised as "a complication of, or the failure in, the technical performance of an indicated procedure or operation", 15.8% as "the failure to synthesise, decide and/or act on available information", 11.8% as "the failure to request or arrange an investigation, procedure or consultation", and 10.9% as "a lack of care and attention or failure to attend the patient". AEs in which the cause was cognitive failure were associated with higher preventability scores than those involving technical performance. The main prevention strategies identified were "new, better, or better implemented policies or protocols" (23.7% of strategies), "more or better formal quality monitoring or assurance processes" (21.2%), "better education and training" (19.2%), and "more consultation with other specialists or peers" (10.2%).
Conclusion: The causes of AEs or errors leading to AEs can be characterised, and human error is a prominent cause. Our study emphasises the need for designing safer systems for care which protect the patient from the inevitability of human error. These systems should provide new policies and protocols and technological support to aid the cognitive activities of clinicians.


Introduction An additional analysis of data from the Quality in Australian Health Care Study (QAHCS)1 was undertaken in order to understand more fully the causes of the adverse events (AEs) identified and to assist in developing prevention strategies. Here, we describe the error or errors in the delivery of healthcare which led to the AEs. This contrasts with our previous report,1 which focused on the patient characteristics associated with AEs and the nature and consequences of the AEs.


Methods The method of determining AEs in the QAHCS has been described previously.1 The AEs were re-examined with the specific goals of determining the causes for, or the underlying errors leading to, each AE. In addition, strategies that were judged to have the potential to prevent AEs were recorded.

To obtain this information the first and subsequent review forms (RF1 and RF2 forms1) collected during the QAHCS were re-examined. The source material for these forms had been the hospital medical records, but neither the hospitals nor the medical records were revisited in this analysis.

Categories for the causes of the AEs were devised by an iterative process during a three-day workshop. For this, we sought additional expertise in clinical epidemiology and qualitative research methods. Using these categories, the AEs recorded on the review forms were assessed by three of the senior medical specialists who had originally reviewed the medical records in the QAHCS.

All the material from each AE was reviewed by only one reviewer, as the agreement between the medical reviewers in determining the presence or absence of an AE during the QAHCS was 80% (kappa, 0.55). A proforma was completed which asked the reviewer to identify the error and then classify it by human cause and preventive strategy. All AEs were also categorised into some of the processes of clinical care. Results for the "delay", "treatment" and "investigation" categories are presented. The categories were not mutually exclusive. These data were then entered into a database, merged with the original data from the QAHCS for each case and analysed. Two of the original total of 2353 AEs were missed in this review; thus, results are given for 2351 AEs.

Our analysis provides the frequency of occurrence of each of the categories of causes of AEs, together with the proportion in each category resulting in permanent disability (including death) and the proportion with high preventability.


Results

Human error categories
Box 2 shows the frequency of occurrence of each of the human error categories, and the proportion of the AEs in each category judged to have permanent disability and high preventability. Of the 2351 AEs, 1922 (81.8%) were associated with one or more human error categories. As the error categories were not mutually exclusive, the 1922 AEs were associated with 2940 causes. "Complication of, or failure in, the technical performance of an indicated procedure/operation" was the most frequent cause of AEs; examples of this category are shown in Box 3A.

Human errors associated with categories of failure of cognitive function were the next most frequent cause of AEs (Box 2). These included "Failure to synthesise, decide and/or act on available information", "Failure to request or arrange investigation, procedure or consultation", and "Misapplication of, or failure to apply, a rule; or use of a bad or inadequate rule".

The most frequent error category, "complication of, or failure in, the technical performance of an indicated procedure/operation", had a lower proportion of AEs with permanent disability (14.2%). The next five most frequent human error categories all had a high proportion of AEs with permanent disability (25% or more) (Box 2). This pattern was also seen in the proportions of AEs with death as the outcome: 2.2% in the first category, and 8% or more in each of the next five categories.

Of the 1201 AEs having high preventability, 9 (0.7%) were not associated with a human error category; for the remaining 1192 AEs, 2051 causes were identified (Box 2).

Delay categories
The importance of timeliness to the quality of healthcare led to further analysis of all AEs to ascertain the nature and role of delay in their causation (Box 4A).

Delays contributed to 20.0% of AEs: of these, delays in diagnosis accounted for 56.8% and treatment delays for 40.6%. Diagnostic delay was usually the failure to make, or attempt to make, a diagnosis of a patient's condition rather than just providing symptomatic or even no treatment. Treatment delay was when the diagnosis had been made but there was a delay in initiating specific therapy. Examples of AEs in the delay category are included in Box 3B.

The AEs with delay categories were judged to have very high preventability (86%-90%) compared with the average (51.2%) for all AEs (Box 4A).

Treatment categories
AEs categorised as caused by a treatment error were also analysed (Box 4B). In 19.6% of all AEs, treatment error contributed to the cause. The majority of AEs in this group fell into the categories of "no or inadequate treatment" (51.5%), or "wrong or inappropriate treatment" (27.4%). As with AEs caused by delay, these AEs were judged to have much higher preventability than the average for all AEs. Examples of AEs involving treatment errors are shown in Box 3C.

Investigation categories
Analysis of the AEs caused by patient investigation issues is shown in Box 4C, and examples are given in Box 3D. There was a problem with clinical investigation in 10.7% of AEs. Paralleling the results in the treatment category, most (78.6%) of these AEs were in this category because an investigation was not done, rather than the investigation being inappropriate (3.6%), or not acted upon (15.5%). Consistent with other AEs that are attributed to cognitive failure, there was a very high percentage of these AEs rated as high preventability.

Strategies for preventing AEs
When describing AEs, preventability refers to the identification of an avoidable error that led to the adverse event. This is not to say that the error could be avoided on every occasion, and that the adverse event would not occur. Rather, it implies that, with the current state of knowledge and technology, it is possible to identify and avoid that particular error, and hence reduce the probability of an AE. The reviewers were making a judgement, having identified the error, on the particular strategy for a change in the healthcare system that could have prevented the AE. The outcomes of these judgements are given in Box 5.

Nineteen (1.6%) of the 1201 high preventability AEs did not have a prevention strategy category. Of the 2613 prevention strategies identified in the 1182 AEs with high preventability, 24.7% (646) were for "better education and training", 20.9% (545) were for "new or better implemented policies or protocols" and 18.6% (486) were for "more or better formal quality monitoring or assurance processes".


Discussion AEs are important to patients, healthcare providers and to the custodians and funders of health services. One estimate of the national cost to the Australian healthcare system of just the additional hospital bed-days (as a result of the AEs identified in 19921) is in excess of $800 million dollars per year.4 This estimate ignores any subsequent hospital admissions and out-of-hospital healthcare expenses, loss of productivity of the patients involved, and long term community costs of permanent disability from AEs. It also ignores the benefits received from healthcare.

Providing insights into how AEs occur can help in developing prevention strategies to reduce the frequency and severity of patient injuries during healthcare. Our review and analysis of the AE data from the QAHCS have shown that the causes of AEs or errors leading to AEs can be characterised, and that human error is a prominent cause.

It is important to recognise that human error is inevitable for even the best-trained and best-qualified healthcare providers. Weed has recently pointed out that the unaided human mind is incapable of performing consistently at the necessary level to provide optimal healthcare.5 However, other studies6 have noted that the label "human error" is prejudicial and non-specific; it may retard rather than advance our understanding of how complex systems fail. It is postulated that within complex systems error is a symptom of organisational problems, and this is likely to apply to healthcare. Therefore, we need a healthcare-system response to error that moves the system towards being as "failsafe" as possible rather than one that blames the clinician who may have erred. Examples from the more frequently studied area of adverse drug events7 would be decision-support technology for antibiotic prescribing,8 with its demonstrated benefits, and electronic prescribing to reduce prescribing and transcription errors in hospital.9

Our analysis identified broad functional categories that are linked to the processes that make up the system of healthcare delivery and hence cut across specialties, diagnosis-related groups (DRGs) and particular patient groups. The sample size is large enough to provide useful information even when several AEs could not be classified into the categories chosen, or insufficient information was available to indicate cause. On the other hand, several factors bias the information available for assessing AEs because of an emphasis on procedures and short term outcomes and possible under-reporting of the contribution of the supporting systems to the cause of the AEs. Firstly, because the original data source was the hospital medical record, the information available about AEs is biased towards the patient involved and away from other potentially important contextual events at the time. Further, the medical record often focuses more on the actions of clinicians involved in direct or procedural patient intervention, and less on the actions of other staff or systems with a more supportive role. These and other factors will lead to an emphasis on procedures and short term outcomes, and a possible under-reporting of the contribution of supporting systems in causing AEs. Finally, information about subsequent or prior hospitalisations is usually only available if the patient attended the same hospital on all occasions.

Having acknowledged these potential limitations, cognitive failure (Box 2) appears to have a role in 57% of all the causes of AEs, and most of the AEs involved were judged to be of high preventability and to have caused significant disability. These AEs were largely associated with errors of omission rather than commission. Does this represent a minimum "obligatory" error rate resulting from a combination of human error and our healthcare system, and hence which cannot be improved? Our data are not able to answer this question unequivocally, but we believe they show sufficient opportunities for moving the system towards a failsafe mode to suggest that the answer is no. Until recently there has been an under-recognition of the role and responsibilities of the healthcare system and its custodians in providing a "safe environment" using systems-improvement tools.10

One response to these data should be to look at the factors in healthcare delivery that may interfere with the cognitive or technical performance of healthcare providers. Insufficient use of information technology to assemble the necessary information at the time of decision-making may increase error. Another important factor is fatigue, which has already been shown to increase error in doctors.11 Sleep deprivation may have a much more significant role in human error in healthcare than the current work-load patterns pay heed to, but more research is needed. Other factors that may be important include the level of supervision provided to junior staff, and the pervasive effect of the culture of medical practice, which can unhelpfully portray error as individual failure or deviation from perfection.12 Our study method does not provide direct information about the role of these factors.

The high proportion of causes of AEs involving cognitive failure must represent a manifestation of human error occurring in a system that is not patient protective, if one accepts that these practitioners are appropriately trained and competent by international standards. Our study provides clear guidance on methods for improvement, with "new, better, or better implemented policies or protocols" accounting for 24% of prevention strategies identified for the AEs, "quality monitoring and assurance processes" accounting for 21%, and "better education and training" for a further 19%.

In summary, improvement is needed in the agreed processes of care, supported by information systems that allow general dissemination of current knowledge of diseases or treatments, and information on outcomes of care for each patient, through appropriate quality processes. Simple examples are the availability of practice guidelines and protocols at the point-of-care, and the use of automated reminders for patients and practitioners when a particular test or follow-up is required. In addition, having adequate patient "outcome" information in a form that can be benchmarked is a powerful tool in identifying unacceptable variation.



Acknowledgements
We acknowledge the contributions of Professor B Armstrong, Professor W R Runciman, Professor R Holland, Dr T Robertson and Dr A Hobbes.


References
  1. Wilson RMcL, Runciman WB, Gibberd RW, et al. The Quality in Australian Health Care Study. Med J Aust 1995; 163: 458-471. <eMJA pdf>
  2. Brennan TA, Leape LL, Laird N, et al. Incidence of adverse events and negligence in hospitalised patients: results of the Harvard Medical Practice Study I. N Engl J Med 1991; 324: 377-384.
  3. Andrews LB, Stocking C, Krizek T, et al. An alternative strategy for studying adverse events in medical care. Lancet 1997; 349: 309-313.
  4. The Final Report of the Taskforce on Quality in Australian Health Care. Appendix 7. Canberra: AGPS, June 1996. <http://www.health.gov.au/pubs/hlthcare/toc.htm>
  5. Weed LL. New connections between medical knowledge and patient care. BMJ 1997; 315: 231-235.
  6. Cook RI, Woods DD. Operating at the sharp end: the complexity of human error. Human performance in anaesthesia: a corpus of cases. Report to the Anaesthesia Patient Safety Foundation, 1991. Columbus, Ohio: The Ohio State University: 255-307.
  7. Classen DC, Pestonick SL, Evans RS, et al. Adverse drug events in hospitalised patients: excess length of stay, extra costs and attributable mortality. JAMA 1997; 227: 301-306.
  8. Evans RS, Pestonick SL, Classen DC, et al. A computer-assisted management program for antibiotics and other anti-infective agents. N Engl J Med 1997; 338: 231-238.
  9. Bates DW, Boyle DL, Vander Vliet MB, et al. Relationship between medication errors and adverse drug events. J Gen Intern Med 1995; 10: 199-205.
  10. Leape LL. A systems analysis approach to medical error. J Eval Clin Pract 1997; 3: 213-222.
  11. Nocera A, Khursandi DS. Doctors' working hours: can the profession afford to let the courts decide what is reasonable. Med J Aust 1998; 168: 616-618.
  12. Leape LL. Error in medicine. JAMA 1994; 272: 1851-1857.

(Received 4 May 1998, accepted 20 Jan 1999)


Authors' details Royal North Shore Hospital, Sydney, NSW 2065.
Ross McL Wilson, MB BS, FRACP, Senior Specialist in Intensive Care; Director of QARNS (Quality Assurance Royal North Shore);
Bernadette T Harrison, RN RM, Manager QARNS.

University of Newcastle, Newcastle, NSW 2308.
Robert W Gibberd, PhD, Associate Professor, Department of Statistics; and Director of Health Services Research Group.
John D Hamilton, MB BS, FRCP, Professor of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences.

Reprints: Dr R McL Wilson, Director of QARNS, Royal North Shore Hospital, Pacific Highway, St Leonards, NSW 2065.
Email: rwilsonATdoh.health.nsw.gov.au




Journalists are welcome to write news stories based on what they read here, but should acknowledge their source as "an article published on the Internet by The Medical Journal of Australia <http://www.mja.com.au>".
<URL: http://www.mja.com.au/>


1: Terms and definitions

Adverse event (AE): An AE was defined as an injury or complication which resulted in disability or prolongation of hospital stay and was caused by the healthcare received rather than by the disease from which the patient suffered. The AE either occurred during the hospital admission, or during an earlier contact with healthcare services, and was responsible for all or part of the hospital admission.
Error: An act of commission or omission that caused, or contributed to the cause of, the unintended injury.

  • for errors of commission this will usually be the immediate morbid consequences of the error
  • for errors of omission this will usually be the continuation of, and consequences of, an existing morbid state that could have been cut short, or had a better outcome, if the error had not occurred.
Prevention strategy: Changes in the system in which an error occurred that may
  • reduce the probability of the error occurring
  • increase the probability that the error would be remedied before an unintentional injury occurred
Preventability: Preventability of an AE was assessed by the detection of "an error in management due to the failure to follow accepted practice at an individual or system level"; accepted practice was taken to be "the current level of expected performance for the average practitioner or system that manages the condition in question".
Back to text

Back to text

3. Examples of categories of causes of adverse events and preventability scores

A. Human error categories
Example 1: A 50-year-old man sustained a bowel perforation from colonoscopy for investigation of abdominal pain. Laparotomy required. Category: "Technical performance".
Preventability score: 3.
Example 2: A 71-year-old man required six operations for femoral hernia repair.
Category: "Technical performance".
Preventability score: 2.
Example 3: A 32-year-old woman had persisting severe back pain after two laminectomies, three myelograms, one decompression/fusion, and three thecal/epidural injections over 18 months. In pelvic traction at the time of review.
Category: "Technical performance".
Preventability score: 4.
Example 4: A failed attempt at percutaneous endoscopic gastrostomy on a 32-year-old woman was followed by an open procedure. The patient died 9 days later. Autopsy revealed acute peritonitis, subphrenic abscess and bilateral pneumonia. There did not appear to have been an antemortem diagnosis of intra-abdominal sepsis, or any specific treatment for it.
Category: "Technical performance".
Preventability score: 6.

B. Delay in diagnosis and/or treatment categories
Example 1: Diagnosis of cancer of the colon was delayed until the patient, a 62-year-old woman, presented with a ruptured caecum and peritonitis from an obstructing tumour. In hospital 3 months earlier with a history suggestive of cancer of the colon and iron-deficiency anaemia, but no investigation performed.
Categories: Diagnosis delay, violation of protocol or rule; failure to synthesise, decide or act on available information; lack of care/attention.
Preventability score: 5.5.
Example 2: A 28-year-old man with abdominal pain was treated with cholecystectomy. Gallbladder was macroscopically and histologically normal. Small bowel lymphoma was eventually diagnosed and treated, with resolution of the presenting symptoms.
Categories: Diagnosis delay; failure to synthesise, decide or act on available information; failure to request or arrange an investigation, procedure or consultation.
Preventability score: 5.

C. Treatment categories
Example 1: A 52-year-old man with known asthma was prescribed a beta-blocker for hypertension. This resulted in acute respiratory failure leading to artifical ventilation and tracheostomy.
Categories: Wrong or inappropriate treatment; misapplication of or failure to apply a rule; failure to synthesise, decide or act on information.
Preventability score: 6.
Example 2: A 54-year-old man developed gastrointestinal bleeding (haemoglobin level, 45 g/L) while receiving non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs and steroids for rheumatoid arthritis. This required hospital admission and blood transfusion, at which time the correct diagnosis of osteoarthritis was made.
Categories: Wrong or inappropriate treatment; acting on insufficient information; failure to request or arrange an investigation, procedure or consultation.
Preventability score: 5.5.
Example 3: Hospitalisation and surgical intervention for septic arthritis that followed steroid injection into a joint.
Categories: Unclassified treatment; technical; lack of care/attention.
Preventability score: 4.5.

D. Investigation categories
Example 1: A 75-year-old woman died from acute renal failure after developing gentamicin toxicity. Gentamicin was used to treat an infected pleural effusion, and drug levels were not measured.
Categories: Investigation not performed; failure to request or arrange an investigation, procedure or consultation; lack of care/attention.
Preventability score: 5.
Example 2: A 58-year-old woman had recurrent hospital admissions for chest pain and impaired cardiac function without specific investigation, and hence a reduction in treatment options.
Categories: Investigation; violation of protocol or rule; failure to synthesise, decide and/or act on available information.
Preventability score: 5.

Back to text

4: Contribution of delay, treatment and investigation categories to adverse events (AEs). Values are number (%) of AEs

PermanentHigh
A. Delay categoryFrequencydisabilitypreventability

Diagnostic delay267 (56.8%)93 (34.8%)231 (86.5%)
Treatment delay191 (40.6%)53 (27.7%)172 (90.1%)
Administrative
delay12 (2.6%)3 (25.0%)11 (91.7%)
Total470 (100%)149 (31.7%)414 (87.9%)
 
B. Treatment category

No or inadequate
treatment237 (51.5%)72 (30.4%)176 (74.3%)
Wrong/inappropriate
treatment126 (27.4%)35 (27.8%)96 (76.2%)
No or inadequate
prophylaxis41 (8.9%)9 (22.0%)34 (82.9%)
Treatment
unclassified36 (7.8%)7 (19.4%)28 (77.8%)
Missed treatment20 (4.4%)4 (20.0%) 16 (80.0%)
Total460 (100%) 127 (27.6%)350 (76.1%)
 
C. Investigation category

Investigation
not performed198 (78.6%)81 (40.9%)171 (86.4%)
Investigation
not acted on39 (15.5%)13 (33.3%)36 (92.3%)
Investigation
inappropriate9 (3.6%)2 (22.2%)9 (100.0%)
Investigation
unclassified6 (2.4%)2 (33.3%)5 (83.3%)
Total252 (100%)98 (38.9%)221 (87.7%)
Back to text

5: Frequency of occurrence of categories of prevention strategies and the proportion of adverse events (AEs) judged as causing permanent disability or having high preventability. Values are number (%) of AEs

PermanentHigh
CategoryFrequencydisabilitypreventability

New, better, or better implemented
policies or protocols884 (23.7%)206 (23.3%)545 (61.7%)
More or better formal quality monitoring
or assurance processes790 (21.2%)186 (23.5%)486 (61.5%)
Better education and training715 (19.2%)160 (22.4%)646 (90.3%)
Consultation with other specialists
or peers391 (10.5%)133 (34.0%)294 (75.2%)
Don't know341 (9.2%)51 (15.0%)186 (54.5%)
Better access to, or transfer of, information135 (3.6%)40 (29.6%)100 (74.1%)
Discharge procedures and protocols122 (3.3%)27 (22.1%)100 (82.0%)
Other89 (2.4%)22 (24.7%)46 (51.7%)
Changes in organisation management88 (2.4%)22 (25.0%)75 (85.2%)
Changes in organisation culture77 (2.1%)26 (33.8%)66 (85.7%)
More or better personnel72 (1.9%)29 (40.3%)53 (73.6%)
More or better equipment or other
physical resources22 (0.6%)8 (36.4%) 16 (72.7%)
Total3726 (100%)*910 (24.4%)2613 (70.1%)

* Total is greater than the number of AEs (2351) as the categories were not mutually exclusive.
Back to text

Received 22 December 2024, accepted 22 December 2024

  • Bernadette T Harrison
  • Robert W Gibberd
  • John D Hamilton



Correspondence: 

Author

remove_circle_outline Delete Author
add_circle_outline Add Author

Comment
Do you have any competing interests to declare? *

I/we agree to assign copyright to the Medical Journal of Australia and agree to the Conditions of publication *
I/we agree to the Terms of use of the Medical Journal of Australia *
Email me when people comment on this article

Online responses are no longer available. Please refer to our instructions for authors page for more information.