In reply: I thank Munro et al, Britton et al, Ryan et al, and Gray et al for their interest in my article and for the opportunity to further highlight complexities in the data which have hindered our understanding of the epidemiology of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID‐19) in children.
Schools remain overlooked as sites of risk. For example, the government of the United Kingdom continues to maintain face masks are unnecessary in these settings,1 despite high levels of community transmission. The household contact studies referred to by Munro and colleagues, are overwhelmingly based on the results of polymerase chain reaction (PCR) tests, and are affected by the biases I previously described.2 However, when serology is assessed, similar secondary attack rates in children, adolescents and adults are observed.3
Munro and colleagues point out mortality is very low in children. This is not in dispute. However, it should be noted that the outcomes they present occurred in the setting of unprecedented non‐pharmaceutical interventions. As such, it would be premature to draw comparisons with influenza. In addition, the long term outcomes of multisystem inflammatory syndrome — a rare but serious complication of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS‐CoV‐2) infection — are unknown.4
Britton et al and Ryan et al highlight a systematic review and meta‐analysis by Viner and colleagues,5 which appears to show children are less likely to be infected than adults. However, as acknowledged by Viner et al, the studies reviewed were predominantly of low to medium quality. Additionally, the authors of the meta‐analysis make analytical errors which further question their findings. With regard to the study by Wang et al,6 Viner and colleagues mistakenly classified adult index cases as contacts. Furthermore, for the study by van der Hoek et al,7 Viner and colleagues erroneously reported zero cases in children when there were at least 24 cases. Furthermore, of the 14 studies included in the meta‐analysis, three did not test all contacts,6,8,9 and in another two10,11 it is unclear if all persons were tested. Two studies included in the meta‐analysis explicitly state non‐high risk contacts were only tested if they developed symptoms.8,9 These limitations are problematic given children are much more likely to be asymptomatic than adults. Importantly, there is evidence that this bias was differential. For example, children were less likely than adults to be tested in the study by Wang et al.6
Similar biases also apply to the review by Goldstein and colleagues,12 referred to by both Munro et al and Ryan et al. It is surprising to note the review’s conclusion, that children aged under 10 years are at most half as susceptible as adults, is based on a single unpublished study. Other studies find children and adults are similarly likely to be infected.3,13 Nonetheless, Goldstein and colleagues12 call for the implementation of risk‐reduction strategies in schools, with which I agree.
It is beyond the scope of this letter to comment on the Journal’s policy with regard to preprints. However, given the rapidly evolving situation in Australia at the time of publication, one would think the timely communication of material that enhanced our understanding of the epidemiology of COVID‐19 would have merit.
Since publication of the Perspective, further studies have provided evidence children may transmit SARS‐CoV‐2 at similar rates to adults. These comprise a study from India of 84 965 cases and 575 071 close contacts14 and a prospective study of 101 households in Tennessee and Wisconsin conducted by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.15 The Victorian data16 referred to by Ryan et al are interesting, but lack precision. While the data indicate children may be less likely to initiate outbreaks in childcare centres than adults (with the caveat that some cases may have gone undetected since not all children may have been tested), no statistically significant difference is observed with regard to primary and secondary schools (Box).
Furthermore, the authors of the Victorian report note 66% of infections in childcare centres and schools were limited to a single case, but fail to note this is an expected consequence of the overdispersed nature of SARS‐CoV‐2 transmission. About 5–20% of primary cases are responsible for 80% of secondary infections, and about 70% of people do not infect anyone.14,17,18 What was seen in Victorian schools is therefore typical for adults and children alike.
Ryan and colleagues describe asymptomatic infection as not uncommon in children. In fact, their own data show it is very common: in Victoria, 33% of children were asymptomatic compared with 17% of adults.16 This is likely to hinder case‐finding. Indeed, the Victorian report notes that “[a]s children tend to be asymptomatic or develop only mild symptoms, it is harder to detect infection in this age group”.16 The report goes on to state that “[a] surveillance strategy that tests only symptomatic children will fail to identify children who are silently shedding the virus, and who may be infectious”.16 Furthermore, the international data referred to by Ryan et al is a single 4–6 week study conducted at a time of low community transmission, comprising only three active cases.19 In contrast, more recent data from Europe show SARS‐CoV‐2 infections in children are frequent. Random testing of the general community in England shows children (aged 5–12 years) and teenagers (aged 13–17 years) are currently more likely to be infected than any other age group.20 Additionally, compared with persons aged 17 years or over, children (aged < 12 years) and adolescents (aged 12–16 years) are now more likely to be the first case in English households, and are more than twice as likely to infect their household contacts.21
I agree school closures are associated with harms. Unfortunately, difficult choices must be made with regard to weighing those harms against the consequences of failing to adequately suppress transmission. Modelling suggests elimination of COVID‐19 in Victoria would have taken longer to achieve if school closures had not been part of the response.22 Morbidity and mortality attributable to COVID‐19 would inevitably have been greater under such conditions.
Gray and colleagues make a valuable point that children have also been overlooked in health education campaigns. Health education and health promotion are vital tools we must deploy against the pandemic. To succeed, we require a diverse, multidisciplinary approach based on a sound understanding of the epidemiology of COVID‐19 in children.
Box – Childcare and school events occurring in Victoria (January–August 2020) and proportion resulting in an outbreak16
Setting and age of first case |
Total number of events |
Additional cases identified |
Proportion resulting in an outbreak (95% CI) |
P |
|||||||||||
No |
Yes |
||||||||||||||
|
|||||||||||||||
Childcare |
|
|
|
|
0.005 |
||||||||||
Child 0–5 years |
54 |
47 |
7 |
13.0% (5.4–24.9%) |
|
||||||||||
Adult ≥ 19 years |
52 |
33 |
19 |
36.5% (23.6–51.0%) |
|
||||||||||
School |
|
|
|
|
0.716 |
||||||||||
Child 6–12 years |
55 |
38 |
17 |
30.9% (19.1–44.8%) |
|
||||||||||
Adolescent 13–15 years |
22 |
13 |
9 |
40.9% (20.7–63.6%) |
|
||||||||||
Adolescent 16–18 years |
75 |
45 |
30 |
40.0% (28.9–52.0%) |
|
||||||||||
Adult ≥ 19 years |
65 |
40 |
25 |
38.5% (26.7–51.4%) |
|
||||||||||
|
|||||||||||||||
P values are for χ2 test. Data are sourced from the report into coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID‐19) in Victorian childcare centres and schools.16 Confidence intervals (CIs) and P values were calculated by the author. Testing was initially restricted to symptomatic persons. In addition, some close contacts may not have been tested. As such, transmission may be underestimated.16 |
- 1. Department of Education, Government of the United Kingdom. Face coverings in education;. last updated 27 Nov 2020 [website]. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/face-coverings-in-education/face-coverings-in-education (viewed Dec 2020).
- 2. Hyde Z. COVID‐19, children and schools: overlooked and at risk. Med J Aust 2020; 213: 444–446. https://www.mja.com.au/journal/2020/213/10/covid-19-children-and-schools-overlooked-and-risk
- 3. Brotons P, Launes C, Buetas E, et al. Susceptibility to SARS‐CoV‐2 infection among children and adults: a seroprevalence study of family households in the Barcelona metropolitan region, Spain. Clin Infect Dis 2020; ciaa1721.
- 4. Jiang L, Tang K, Levin M, et al. COVID‐19 and multisystem inflammatory syndrome in children and adolescents. Lancet Infect Dis 2020; 20: e276–e288.
- 5. Viner RM, Mytton OT, Bonell C, et al. Susceptibility to SARS‐CoV‐2 infection among children and adolescents compared with adults: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. JAMA Pediatr 2020;. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.4573. [Epub ahead of print].
- 6. Wang Z, Ma W, Zheng X, et al. Household transmission of SARS‐CoV‐2. J Infect 2020; 81: 179–182.
- 7. van der Hoek W, Backer JA, Bodewes R, et al. The role of children in the transmission of SARS‐CoV‐2] [Dutch]. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd 2020; 164: D5140.
- 8. Cheng HY, Jian SW, Liu DP, et al. Contact tracing assessment of COVID‐19 transmission dynamics in Taiwan and risk at different exposure periods before and after symptom onset. JAMA Intern Med 2020; 180: 1156–1163.
- 9. Park YJ, Choe YJ, Park O, et al. Contact tracing during coronavirus disease outbreak, South Korea, 2020. Emerg Infect Dis 2020; 26: 2465–2468.
- 10. Wang Y, Tian H, Zhang L, et al. Reduction of secondary transmission of SARS‐CoV‐2 in households by face mask use, disinfection and social distancing: a cohort study in Beijing, China. BMJ Glob Health 2020; 5: e002794.
- 11. Mizumoto K, Omori R, Nishiura H. Age specificity of cases and attack rate of novel coronavirus disease (COVID‐19) [preprint]. medRxiv 2020.03.09.20033142. 13 Mar 2020. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.03.09.20033142 (viewed Dec 2020).
- 12. Goldstein E, Lipsitch M, Cevik M. On the effect of age on the transmission of SARS‐CoV‐2 in households, schools and the community. J Infect Dis 2020; jiaa691.
- 13. Bi Q, Wu Y, Mei S, et al. Epidemiology and transmission of COVID‐19 in 391 cases and 1286 of their close contacts in Shenzhen, China: a retrospective cohort study. Lancet Infect Dis 2020; 20: 911–919.
- 14. Laxminarayan R, Wahl B, Dudala SR, et al. Epidemiology and transmission dynamics of COVID‐19 in two Indian states. Science 2020; 370: 691–697.
- 15. Grijalva CG, Rolfes MA, Zhu Y, et al. Transmission of SARS‐COV-2 infections in households — Tennessee and Wisconsin, April–September 2020. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 2020; 69: 1631–1634.
- 16. Russell F, Ryan KE, Snow K, et al. COVID‐19 in Victorian schools: an analysis of child‐care and school outbreak data and evidence‐based recommendations for opening schools and keeping them open. Melbourne, Australia: Murdich Children’s Research Institute and University of Melbourne, 2020. https://www.mcri.edu.au/sites/default/files/media/covid_in_schools_report_final_10112020.pdf (viewed Dec 2020).
- 17. Endo A, Abbott S, Kucharski AJ, et al. Estimating the overdispersion in COVID‐19 transmission using outbreak sizes outside China. Wellcome Open Res 2020; 5: 67.
- 18. Adam DC, Wu P, Wong JY, et al. Clustering and superspreading potential of SARS‐CoV‐2 infections in Hong Kong. Nat Med 2020; 26: 1714–1719.
- 19. Public Health England. COVID‐19 surveillance in school KIDs (sKIDs): pre and primary schools. London: Department of Health and Social Care, Government of the United Kingdom, 2020. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/914700/sKIDs_Phase1Report_01sep2020.pdf (viewed Dec 2020).
- 20. Riley S, Walters CE, Wang H, et al. REACT‐1 round 7 updated report: regional heterogeneity in changes in prevalence of SARS‐CoV‐2 infection during the second national COVID‐19 lockdown in England [preprint]. medRxiv 2020.12.15.20248244. 16 Dec 2020. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.15.20248244 (viewed Dec 2020).
- 21. Scientific Advisory Group for Emergencies. Children’s Task and Finish Group: update to 4 November 2020 paper on children, schools and transmission. London: Government of the United Kingdom, 2020. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/tfc-children-and-transmission-update-paper-17-december-2020 (viewed Jan 2021).
- 22. Blakely T, Thompson J, Carvalho N, et al. The probability of the 6‐week lockdown in Victoria (commencing 9 July 2020) achieving elimination of community transmission of SARS‐CoV‐2. Med J Aust 2020; 213: 349–351. https://www.mja.com.au/journal/2020/probability-6-week-lockdown-victoria-commencing-9-july-2020-achieving-elimination
No relevant disclosures.