Connect
MJA
MJA

Randomised controlled trial of graded exercise in chronic fatigue syndrome

Karen E Wallman, Alan R Morton, Carmel Goodman, Robert Grove and Andrew M Guilfoyle
Med J Aust 2004; 180 (9): 444-448. || doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2004.tb06019.x
Published online: 3 May 2004

Abstract

Objective: To investigate whether 12 weeks of graded exercise with pacing would improve specific physiological, psychological and cognitive functions in people with chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS).

Design: Randomised controlled trial.

Setting: Human performance laboratory at the University of Western Australia.

Participants: 61 patients aged between 16 and 74 years diagnosed with CFS.

Interventions: Either graded exercise with pacing (32 patients) or relaxation/flexibility therapy (29 patients) performed twice a day over 12 weeks.

Main outcome measures: Changes in any of the physiological, psychological or cognitive variables assessed.

Results: Following the graded exercise intervention, scores were improved for resting systolic blood pressure (P = 0.018), work capacity (W·kg-1) (P = 0.019), net blood lactate production (P = 0.036), depression (P = 0.027) and performance on a modified Stroop Colour Word test (P = 0.029). Rating of perceived exertion scores, associated with an exercise test, was lower after graded exercise (P = 0.013). No such changes were observed in the relaxation/flexibility condition, which served as an attention-placebo control.

Conclusions: Graded exercise was associated with improvements in physical work capacity, as well as in specific psychological and cognitive variables. Improvements may be associated with the abandonment of avoidance behaviours.

Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) is a common disorder that, despite intense research, still has an elusive aetiology, no conclusive laboratory marker, and no established cure. Graded exercise therapy and cognitive behavioural therapy are two management techniques that have consistently helped patients with CFS.1 However, of the three randomised controlled trials that assessed graded exercise treatment,2-4 none accounted for the cyclical nature of symptoms.5,6 It is unclear whether the benefits reported reflected the outcome of the treatment, the fluctuating nature of the syndrome, or a combination of both. Additionally, two of the controlled trials used maximal oxygen consumption testing to assess physical function in CFS subjects.2,4 This parameter is generally recognised as the gold standard for assessing cardiopulmonary function.7 However, issues arise as to the suitability of using an exercise test that requires maximal effort from people with debilitating fatigue, particularly when this fatigue is exacerbated by physical activity.8-10 Use of maximal oxygen consumption testing may deter some people with CFS from participating in trials and encourage the more robust or healthier subjects to volunteer for testing. This selection may influence the representativeness of the population with CFS in trials.

Our aims were:

  • To confirm or refute the outcomes of previous trials that reported physiological and psychological improvements associated with graded exercise in CFS subjects.2-4

  • To determine whether a 12-week program of graded exercise would result in a significant improvement in attentional function in CFS sufferers. To date, no study has assessed the effects of graded exercise on cognitive function in CFS.

Methods
Recruitment

CFS subjects were recruited from notices placed in medical surgeries and by advertisements in local newspapers. Eighty-two patients (aged 16–74 years) applied to participate. Before participation, written confirmation of a CFS diagnosis, as defined by Fukuda et al,11 was required from each subject’s doctor. Fourteen applicants were excluded because of alternative diagnoses or failure to provide written confirmation of diagnosis. The remaining 68 patients were randomised (by an independent investigator) to a graded exercise or a relaxation/flexibility intervention. Six subjects withdrew from baseline testing for reasons not associated with the study, and a further subject was excluded because her body mass index (44 kg/m2) prevented her from participating in the exercise test. This left 32 CFS subjects in the graded exercise group (27 women, 5 men), and 29 CFS subjects in the relaxation/flexibility group (20 women, 9 men) (Box 1).

Based on an assumption of moderate effect size (0.4–0.6), a sample size of at least 30 subjects was needed in each intervention group to detect an effect at an α of 0.05 with 80% power.12

All subjects were assessed for depression as defined by the Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders13 using a computerised version of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview.14

Assessment

For the 4 weeks before and 4 weeks after the intervention, subjects attended weekly testing sessions at a university laboratory, on the same weekday at the same time. These assessment results were averaged for each 4-week period. The same exercise physiologist conducted all tests.

Resting heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP) were first recorded, then subjects were assessed on the Aerobic Power Index test,15 a submaximal cycle test that has been shown to be reliable in a CFS population (intraclass correlation coefficient, 0.97).16 This exercise test starts at low power and increases in small power increments until an individual target heart rate (THR: 220 – age × 0.75) is reached.15 The power output adjusted for body weight (W·kg-1) that coincided with a subject’s THR was determined through interpolation techniques described by Telford and Minikin.17 If a subject failed to reach the THR, the W·kg-1 achieved during the last full minute of exercise was recorded.

Ratings of perceived exertion (RPE), as measured by the Borg scale,18 were recorded 55 seconds into each minute of the exercise test. Oxygen consumption was analysed continuously during the exercise test by a metabolic cart consisting of a computerised online system. Blood lactate was measured before and 3 minutes after the exercise test.

A similar interpolation procedure to that used to calculate power output at THR was used to determine RPE, oxygen uptake and respiratory exchange ratio values that coincided with each individual’s THR. RPE values that equated to the subject’s THR were divided by the final workload (W·kg-1) achieved either at or before THR. This ratio represents perceived exertion sensed at the end of the exercise test adjusted for the actual work done.

Weekly energy expenditure was determined using the Older Adult Exercise Status Inventory.19 This scale excludes activities involving sitting or lying down.

Mental and physical fatigue was assessed using a 14-item self-rating scale,20 and anxiety and depression were rated by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.21

Cognitive function was tested using a computerised version of the modified Stroop Colour Word test. This test has been used previously to assess attentional function in CFS.22-24 We used two levels of difficulty based on speed of presentation.

On completion of the 12-week intervention, subjects indicated whether they felt the program had benefited them, using a global impression scale25 from 1 (“Very much better”) to 7 (“Very much worse”).

Statistical analysis

Preliminary screening of the data indicated that all continuous variables were normally distributed. Analyses were conducted using 2 (group) × 2 (time) mixed-model ANOVAs, with repeated measures on the time factor.27 Significant interaction effects were examined further by post hoc comparison of group differences using F tests. Differences in ability to reach target HR during the exercise test were assessed using a Mann–Whitney non-parametric test. Results for the self-rated clinical global impression change score were collapsed in a 2 × 2 contingency table and compared using a χ2 test corrected for continuity. All statistical procedures were performed using SPSS.28

Results

There were no initial differences between the treatment and control groups in age, body mass, height, activity levels or illness duration.

Six subjects were classified as having had a major depressive disorder in the previous 12 months (single episodes that ranged from mild to severe without psychotic features). Two other subjects were classified with dysthymia. Results presented include data from all subjects, because analyses that excluded data from the six subjects diagnosed with a previous major depressive disorder produced similar results.

Physiological results

Physiological results are shown in Box 2 and Box 3. Repeated-measures ANOVAs showed significant treatment by time interactions for all variables except resting diastolic BP.

We observed a significant improvement in resting systolic BP (P = 0.018) and in W·kg-1 (P = 0.019) in the exercise group. Improvement in W·kg-1 was reflected by a significant increase in net lactate production (P = 0.036), as well as by the number of exercise tests on which THR was achieved by the exercise group during post-intervention testing (P = 0.030) (Box 4). Oxygen uptake values were 9.6% higher after the intervention in the exercise group compared with an 8.9% decline in the relaxation/flexibility group, but the difference in final values for the groups was not significant. Of interest, ratings of perceived exertion were lower after the exercise intervention (P = 0.013). Activity levels increased in the graded exercise group, although the final levels did not differ between the groups (Box 2).

Psychological results

Mean psychological scores are shown in Box 5. Post-hoc analysis showed that, while baseline depression scores were similar in both groups, scores were significantly lower in the exercise group following the intervention (P = 0.027). While there were no other significant post-intervention differences, follow-up tests showed a significant improvement in mental fatigue over time in the exercise group (P < 0.001).

On the modified Stroop Colour Word test, there were no significant differences between the groups before the intervention, but a significant difference in favour of the graded exercise group after the intervention on the more difficult level of this test (P = 0.029) (Box 5).

Self-rated impression of benefit

Box 6 shows the scores on the self-rated clinical global impression change scale. Although there was no significant difference between the two groups (χ2 = 1.46; df = 1; P = 0.227), a greater percentage of subjects in the exercise group rated themselves as being better (exercise group, 29/32 [91%]; relaxation group, 22/29 [76%]).

Discussion

Our physiological results support those reported by others,2,4 in that a graded exercise program resulted in a significant improvement in work capacity (W·kg-1). The ability to do more work after graded exercise was also reflected by significantly higher net lactate production and by a significant improvement (6.7%) in the ability of subjects in this group to reach their THR. This improved ability to do more work may be because the exercise regimen was not associated with any relapse. This may have resulted in the abandonment of the commonly held belief by CFS sufferers that exercise results in the exacerbation of symptoms, which consequently leads to the avoidance of exercise.29,30 Additionally, graded exercise was associated with a reduction in perceived effort scores associated with the exercise test. Many studies have documented that CFS subjects report a greater sense of effort associated with exercise tests compared with control subjects.9,31,32 Gibson et al9 suggest that an abnormal sense of effort in CFS may be the result of the resetting of a sensory threshold in response to the initial onset of symptoms, and that this then becomes a learned response. Results from our study suggest that 12 weeks of graded exercise intervention may have resulted in a further resetting of this sensory threshold toward more normal values. Of importance is the implication that this reduced sense of effort may be associated with everyday work tasks that require similar work capacities.

Scores for depression improved after graded exercise, and mental fatigue decreased. Reduced symptoms of fatigue after graded exercise intervention have been previously reported.2,4 These psychological improvements may be due to improved physical capacity and the potential of aerobic exercise to increase core body temperature, which can reduce muscle tension and stress33 and consequently improve sleep patterns.34 Reduced psychological symptoms may also result in attenuated afferent feedback to the motor cortex in CFS subjects, which can subsequently reduce effort sensation.35

Our graded exercise intervention was also associated with an improvement in attentional function. To date, little has been reported about the effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive performance. Christodoulou et al36 reported an association between inactivity and increased errors made on neuropsychological tests by CFS subjects, and Vercoulen et al37 reported a relationship between low levels of activity and impaired cognitive function in CFS subjects, demonstrated by slowed speed of information processing. It has been proposed that lack of physical and cognitive practice reduces automatic function in these areas in CFS sufferers, which makes physical and cognitive tasks more effortful.31,38 Furthermore, it has been postulated that a graded return to cognitive and physical activities may assist CFS sufferers in regaining automaticity in these functions, as well as reduce the sense of effort associated with these tasks.31

Most subjects (91%) rated themselves as feeling better after the exercise intervention, and no subject felt that graded exercise worsened their symptoms.

Limitations of the study relate to the use of a relaxation/flexibility control group rather than a control group that experienced no intervention, and neither subjects nor the exercise physiologist were blinded to the intervention. Additionally, 12 weeks may not have been long enough to maximise changes. Further studies that include a longer intervention period and a formal follow-up would provide valuable information in this debilitating disorder.

Conclusion

Our study showed that a graded exercise intervention that included a form of pacing was associated with specific physiological, psychological and cognitive improvements as compared with a control regimen of relaxation/flexibility. An important aspect of our study was that graded exercise was not associated with a relapse in any participant. While not a cure for CFS, graded exercise improves functional ability, and minimises deconditioning, which can result in more symptoms.

Received 16 September 2003, accepted 8 December 2003

  • Karen E Wallman1
  • Alan R Morton2
  • Carmel Goodman3
  • Robert Grove4
  • Andrew M Guilfoyle5

  • 1 School of Human Movement and Exercise Science, University of Western Australia, Perth, WA.
  • 2 School of Psychology, Edith Cowan University, Perth, WA.


Correspondence: 

Competing interests:

None identified.

  • 1. Whiting P, Bagnall AM, Sowden AJ, et al. Interventions for the treatment and management of chronic fatigue syndrome: a systematic review. JAMA 2001; 286: 1360-1368.
  • 2. Fulcher KY, White PD. Randomised controlled trial of graded exercise in patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome. BMJ 1997; 314: 1647-1652.
  • 3. Powell R, Bentall R, Nye FJ, Edwards RHT. Randomised controlled trial of patient education to encourage graded exercise in chronic fatigue syndrome. BMJ 2001; 322: 387-390.
  • 4. Weardon A, Morris R, Mullis R, et al. Randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled treatment trial of fluoxetine and graded exercise for chronic fatigue syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 1998; 172: 485-490.
  • 5. McCluskey DR. Chronic fatigue syndrome and its effect on exercise capacity. In: Bock G, Whalan J editors. Chronic fatigue syndrome, ciba foundation symposium No. 173. London: John Wiley and Sons, 1993: 239-250.
  • 6. Pheby D. Chronic fatigue syndrome: a challenge to the clinical professions. Physiotherapy 1997; 83: 53-56.
  • 7. McCully K, Sisto S, Natelson B. Use of exercise for treatment of chronic fatigue syndrome. Sports Med 1996; 21: 35-48.
  • 8. Lapp CW. Exercise limits in chronic fatigue syndrome. Am J Med 1997; 103: 83-84.
  • 9. Gibson H, Carroll N, Clague J, Edwards RHT. Exercise performance and fatiguability in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993; 56: 993-998.
  • 10. LaManca J, Sisto SA, Zhou X, et al. Immunological response in chronic fatigue syndrome following a graded exercise test to exhaustion. J Clin Immunol 1999; 19: 135-142.
  • 11. Fukuda K, Straus S, Hickie I, et al. The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Ann Intern Med 1994; 121: 953-959.
  • 12. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull 1992; 112: 155-159.
  • 13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders (fourth edition). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1994.
  • 14. Peters L, Andrews G. A procedural validity study of the computerised version of the composite international diagnostic interview. Psychol Med 1995; 25: 1269-1280.
  • 15. Telford RD, Minikin BR, Hahn AG, Hooper LA. A simple method for the assessment of general fitness: the tri-level profile. Aust J Sci Med Sport 1989; 21: 6-9.
  • 16. Wallman K, Morton A, Goodman C, et al. Reliability studies of the aerobic power index submaximal exercise test in a CFS population. J Chronic Fatigue Syndr (in press).
  • 17. Telford RD, Minikin BR. The tri-level test for runners — a simple method of general fitness evaluation. Excel 1989; 6: 33-36.
  • 18. Borg GA. Psychophysical basis of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1982; 14: 377-381.
  • 19. O’Brien Cousins S. An older adult exercise status inventory: reliability and validity. J Sport Behav 1996; 19: 288-302.
  • 20. Chalder T, Berelowitz G, Pawlikowska T, et al. Development of a fatigue scale. J Psychosom Res 1993; 17: 147-153.
  • 21. Zigmond A, Snaith R. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Physiol Scand 1983; 87: 361-370.
  • 22. Schmaling K, Di Clementi J, Culllum M, Jones J. Cognitive functioning in chronic fatigue syndrome and depression: a preliminary comparison. Psychosom Med 1994; 5: 383-388.
  • 23. Brickman AL, Fins AI. Psychological and cognitive aspects of chronic fatigue syndrome. In: Goodnik PJ, Klimas NG, editors. Chronic fatigue and related immune deficiency syndromes. Progress in psychiatry. No. 40. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1993: 47-93.
  • 24. Funetes K, Hunter MA, Strauss E, Hultsch DF. Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in persons with chronic fatigue syndrome. Clin Neuropsychol 2001; 15: 210-227.
  • 25. Guy W. ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health, 1976: 218-222.
  • 26. Shepherd C. Pacing and exercise in chronic fatigue. Physiotherapy 2001; 87: 395-396.
  • 27. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 3rd ed. New York: HarperCollins, 1996.
  • 28. SPSS [computer program]. Version 11.0. Chicago, Ill: SPSS Inc, 1996.
  • 29. Clement A, Sharpe M, Simkin S, et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome: a qualitative investigation of patients: beliefs about the illness. J Psychosom Res 1997; 42: 615-624.
  • 30. Edwards R, Suresh R, Lynch S, et al. Illness perceptions and mood in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Psychosom Res 2001; 50: 65-68.
  • 31. Blackwood S, MacHale S, Power M, et al. Effects of exercise on cognitive and motor function in chronic fatigue syndrome and depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998; 65: 541-546.
  • 32. Kent-Braun J, Sharma K, Weiner M, et al. Central basis of muscle fatigue in chronic fatigue syndrome. Neurology 1993; 43: 125-131.
  • 33. DeVries HA. Tranquilizer effect of exercise: a critical review. Phys Sportsmed 1981; 9: 47-55.
  • 34. Sharpe M, Wessely S. Putting the rest cure to rest again: rest has no place in treating chronic fatigue. BMJ 1998; 316: 796.
  • 35. Lawrie S, McHale S, Power M, Goodwin G. Is the chronic fatigue syndrome best understood as a primary disturbance of the sense of effort? [editorial]. Psychol Med 1997; 27: 995-999.
  • 36. Christodoulou C, DeLuca J, Lange G, et al. Relation between neuropsychological impairment and functional disability in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998; 64: 431-434.
  • 37. Vercoulen JH, Bazelmans E, Swanink CM, et al. Evaluating neuropsychological impairment in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1998; 20: 144-156.
  • 38. Joyce E, Blumenthal S, Wessely S. Memory, attention, and executive function in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1996; 60: 495-503.

Author

remove_circle_outline Delete Author
add_circle_outline Add Author

Comment
Do you have any competing interests to declare? *

I/we agree to assign copyright to the Medical Journal of Australia and agree to the Conditions of publication *
I/we agree to the Terms of use of the Medical Journal of Australia *
Email me when people comment on this article

Online responses are no longer available. Please refer to our instructions for authors page for more information.