Research
Use of fake tanning lotions in the South Australian population
Kerri R Beckmann, Barbara A Kirke, Kieran A McCaul and David M Roder
MJA 2001; 174: 75-78
Abstract -
Methods -
Results -
Discussion -
Conclusions -
References -
Authors' Details
-
-
More articles on Public and environmental health
|
Objective: To explore the relationship between the
use of fake tanning lotions and repeated sunburn among South
Australian adults, with a view to informing the Anti-Cancer
Foundation of South Australia's (ACFSA) policy on fake tanning
products. Study design: Population survey. Participants: 2005 South Australians aged 18 years or
older, selected randomly from the electronic White Pages. Main outcome measures: Self-reported use of fake tanning
lotions in the past 12 months; frequency of sunburn over summer; and
various sun-protective behaviours. Results: 2005 of the 2536 eligible participants (79%)
were surveyed by telephone. Fake tan use was most prevalent among
women (15.9%), people aged 18-24 years (15.4%), and people with
household incomes above $40 000 per year (11.9%). Fake tan users were
more likely than non-users to use sunscreens (81.3% v 56.5%; P
< 0.001), but less likely to take other precautions such as wearing
hats (40.9% v 51.0%; P = 0.04) and protective clothing (22.3% v
34.1%; P = 0.005). They were also more likely to report having
been burnt more than once over summer (26.2% v 16.5%; P =
0.025). Multivariate analysis indicates a statistically
significant association between fake tan use and repeated sunburn
(odds ratio, 2.07; 95% confidence interval, 1.17-3.69), which was
independent of age, sex, skin type and sun-protection
practices. Conclusion: Users of fake tanning products may be at
greater risk of repeated sunburn. The ACFSA sees no justification at
this stage for altering its present policy position of not actively
promoting the use of fake tanning lotions as a means of reducing
sunburn.
|
| |
Anticancer organisations in Australia have been conducting
programs aimed at reducing Australia's high rate of skin cancer for
over two decades.1 The main objective of these
programs is to encourage people to reduce their exposure to solar
ultraviolet radiation, the major contributing factor to the
development of skin cancer.2 In Australia public awareness about the dangers of overexposure to
the sun is generally high. In spite of this, a suntan is still desired by
some sectors of the community -- in particular, young,
fashion-conscious people.3,4 Skin cancer prevention
programs have attempted to change attitudes that value tanned skin as
attractive and healthy with such messages as "there is no such thing as
a safe tan" and "a tan is a sign of skin damage".
Last year, Chapman challenged anticancer organisations to consider
the role that fake tanning lotions might play in reducing sun
exposure, suggesting that they should be assessed as a potential
harm-reduction strategy.5
The Anti-Cancer Foundation of South Australia (ACFSA) has, for a
number of years, provided information on fake tanning lotions. While
not actively encouraging their use, the information suggests that,
for those desiring a tan, using fake tanning lotions is preferable to
exposure to artificial or solar ultraviolet radiation.
In October 1999, the ACFSA included a question on the use of fake
tanning lotions in a Health Monitor Survey along with questions on
skin type, experience of sunburn and frequency of wearing hats,
cover-up clothing, applying sunscreen and seeking shade.
This article reports the findings of that survey and discusses them in
relation to the position taken by the ACFSA regarding fake tanning
lotions.
|
| Methods |
Questions relating to sun exposure and ultraviolet radiation
protective behaviours, including one relating to the use of fake
tanning lotions, were asked of a random sample of South Australians,
by computer-assisted telephone interviewing. These questions (Box
1) were part of a larger health-related survey organised and
conducted by the Department of Human Services, South Australia, in
October 1999. Except for the question relating to fake tan use, these
questions have been used routinely in monitoring sun-protection
behaviours in South Australia and were originally validated as
written questions in a national Secondary School Children's Survey
conducted triennially since 1990.6 The question on fake tan use
is a slightly modified version of a question asked in Victorian
surveys in 1993 and 1995.7,8 Ethical approval for the survey that incorporated the questions used
in this study was obtained through the Department of Human Services,
with legal authorisation under section 64d of the South
Australian Health Commission Act (1976).
A sample of 3400 residences from rural and metropolitan areas within
South Australia was drawn from the electronic White Pages. One adult
from each household (the person whose birthday was the most recent)
was invited to participate. Two thousand and five interviews were
conducted from the 2536 households that could be contacted after six
callback attempts, giving a participation rate of 79.1%.
All data were weighted by age, sex and region, and on the probability of
selection within the household. The population profile for
weighting was obtained from the Australian Bureau of Statistics'
estimated population for South Australia, 1997. Geographical
region was defined as either metropolitan or country region. Both
descriptive analysis of the survey data and logistic regression
modelling were undertaken using STATA version 6,9 as this software
allows calculation of robust estimates of standard error using
methods devised by Huber10 and White.11
Consequently, variance estimates are adjusted for the data
weighting.
The relationship between fake tan use and reported sunburn over
summer was examined using logistic regression modelling, allowing
adjustment for age, sex, skin type and sun-protective behaviours. We
first constructed a model containing known risk factors for sunburn
and then added fake tan use to this model to establish if this improved
the fit of the model. "Having been burnt two or more times during the
previous summer" was the dependent (outcome) variable. For each of
the sun protective behaviour questions, respondents were coded as
regular users if they indicated that they "usually, almost always, or
always" took such precautions when out in the sun for an hour or more,
and were coded as irregular users if they indicated that they "never,
rarely or sometimes" took these measures.
|
| |
Based on results from this survey, the estimated prevalence of fake
tanning lotion use during the past 12 months among South Australians
aged 18 years or more was 8.7% (95% confidence interval [CI],
7.3%-10.5%). The prevalence of fake tan use among various subgroups
of the population is shown in Box 2. The use of fake tanning lotions is
most common among younger people, particularly women, with the peak
prevalence being 28% among young women aged 18-24 years. Fake tan use
is also more common among those who report that their skin burns before
tanning, compared with those whose skin just tans or just burns. Fake
tan use also appears to be related to household income, with those with
relatively high household incomes (above $40 000 per year) more
likely to use fake tanning lotions.
Individuals who had used fake tanning lotions in the past year were
more likely to report regularly using sunscreen with a sun protection
factor (SPF) of 15+ or higher when in the sun than non-users (81% v 57%;
P < 0.001). However, they were less likely to report
regularly wearing hats (41% v 51%; P = 0.04) or protective
clothing (22% v 34%; P = 0.005). They reported seeking shade at
levels similar to those who had not used fake tanning lotions (80% v
76%, P = 0.4). Those who had used fake tanning lotions were also
more likely to report having been burnt two or more times during the
previous summer (26% v 17%; P = 0.025) (Box 3).
Factors such as age, skin type, sex and regular sun-protective
behaviours are likely to confound the association between fake tan
use and risk of burning. Results of logistic regression modelling,
which takes into account the effects of these potential confounders,
indicate an increased risk among fake tan users of having been
sunburnt more than once (odds ratio [OR], 2.07; 95% CI, 1.17-3.69), as
shown in Box 4.
As the use of fake tanning lotions was much more prevalent among women
than men, we also undertook regression analyses for women and men
separately. No association between fake tan use and sunburn was found
among men (OR, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.14-5.97). This was most probably owing
to the fact that only 12 men reported using fake tanning lotions. There
was, however, a strong association between using fake tanning
lotions and repeated sunburn among women (OR, 2.47; 95% CI,
1.38-4.42).
|
| Discussion |
While the overall prevalence of fake tan use among adult South
Australians is low (8.7%; 95% CI, 7.3%-10.5%), the use of fake tanning
lotions is fairly common in younger women, with more than one in four
women aged 18-24 years reporting having used fake tanning lotions in
the past year. These findings are consistent with the reported
prevalence of use in Victoria.7,8 Respondents who reported using fake tanning lotions were more likely
to report regularly using SPF 15+ or higher sunscreen when out in the
sun during summer, but were less likely to report wearing hats or
protective clothing. Fake tan users were more likely to report being
sunburnt two or more times over the past summer. When other known risk
factors were taken into account, fake tan users had twice the risk of
repeated sunburn over summer compared with non-users.
The only previously reported findings in relation to the association
between fake tan use and sunburn are from two surveys conducted in
Victoria, one in 19937 and one in 1995.8 The first of
these surveys found a higher prevalence of sunburn among fake tan
users (66% v 46%), while the latter survey found no difference (39% v
40%). The inconsistency of these two reports may have been owing to the
relatively small sample size of each survey (n < 700).
Owing to the limited nature of the questions in this survey, we were
unable to determine whether fake tanning lotions were used just at the
start of the season to give a tanned look before a sun-induced tan could
be achieved, or throughout the summer as a substitute for sunbathing.
Given the timing of the survey (ie, spring), there may be some
inaccuracy in people's recall of sunburn in the previous summer.
However, it seems unlikely to us that one group would have been more or
less likely to under-report having been sunburnt, so any recall
effect would have been equivalent in both groups.
Another limitation in relation to the timing and cross-sectional
nature of the survey is the inability to establish a temporal
relationship. In some cases, sunburn may have preceded the use of fake
tanning lotions. We can not conclude that fake tan use contributes
directly to an increased risk of sunburn. We can only suggest that the
behaviours of fake tan use and sun exposure may be linked.
A further limitation of this study is that we did not ask about the
reason for or frequency of use. We do not know whether there are
differences in the risk of sunburn among those who use fake tanning
lotions only on special occasions (eg, theatrical performances)
compared with those who use such products regularly to maintain a
tanned appearance.
This lack of detail does not negate the finding that, as a whole, those
who use fake tanning lotions are at greater risk of sunburn.
Regardless of when and why people use fake tanning lotions, the
results of this survey do not offer any evidence that use of fake
tanning lotions, as currently practised, protects against sunburn.
However, since this is an observational study, we can not rule out the
possibility that the use of fake tanning lotions may actually offer
some protection. It is conceivable that, had users not been applying
fake tanning lotions, sunburn levels could have been even higher in
this group. Further clarification of this issue would require a
longitudinal (experimental) study design.
Our results suggest that, rather than reducing their sun exposure,
fake tan users are more likely to be exposing their skin to damaging
levels of ultraviolet radiation than non-users. The evidence also
suggests that, in general, fake tan users take fewer precautions to
protect their skin from the sun. While fake tan users are more likely to
report using sunscreens, they appear to rely on sunscreens alone for
sun protection rather than using multiple strategies, as
recommended by the Anti-Cancer Foundation. Some fake tan users may
believe that the tanned effect provided by fake tanning lotions
offers protection against the sun. Further confusion may arise in
cases where their chosen brand of tanning lotion contains sunscreen.
An inspection of fake tanning lotions currently available in South
Australia showed that most brands do not include a sunscreen, and many
state on the label that the fake tan does not provide protection
against solar ultraviolet radiation. Some brands also include the
advice to use a regular SPF 30+ sunscreen when going into the sun.
However, a few brands of fake tanning lotions do contain sunscreen,
varying in their sun protection factor from 4 to 15. One commonly
available product with an SPF 4 rating states on its label, "UV
Protection: Protects you in the sun, providing 4 times your natural
sunburn protection". While technically correct, the protection
would apply only to the period immediately after application and not
for the time that the tan remains visible on the skin. Such claims are
obviously very misleading.
Anticancer organisations advise that sunscreens need to be
reapplied regularly, ideally two-hourly, to maintain adequate
protection. If such advice was followed when using a fake tanning
lotion containing a sunscreen, the colour of the tan would deepen with
each application. Also, it may take up to four hours for the tan colour
to fully develop. Most products recommend removal of dry or flaky
skin before applying the fake tanning lotion. The need to do
this and the effect of repeated applications on tan colour are likely
to preclude regular reapplication of fake tanning lotions, thereby
increasing the likelihood of users risking sunburn if they rely on the
protection offered by one application. In our view, including a
sunscreen in a fake tanning lotion offers no obvious benefit. On the
contrary, it has the potential to generate a false sense of protection
which may lead to sunburn in fake tan users.
|
| Conclusions |
In response to the discussion posed by Chapman5 in relation to the use of fake
tanning lotions as a "harm minimisation" approach, the ACFSA was
prompted to review its policy in relation to the promotion and sale of
fake tanning lotions.
The results of this study do not point to a reduced risk of harmful sun
exposure among fake tan users. Rather, they suggest an elevated risk
of sunburn. In the light of these findings, the ACFSA sees no
justification for altering its current position on the use of fake
tanning lotions. The use of fake tanners is not actively promoted by
the ACFSA. However, where there is a strong desire for a tan, people are
advised that the use of fake tanning lotions is a better alternative
than sunbathing or using a solarium. They are also advised that a fake
tan does not provide protection against the sun and are warned about
the limited protection offered by products that contain a sunscreen.
More in-depth investigation of why and when fake tanning lotions are
used, and the extent to which fake tan users believe they are protected
from the harmful effects of the sun while using such products, is
needed to inform education strategies and guide any policy change by
organisations such as the ACFSA. The potential of the labelling of
fake tan products containing sunscreens to be misleading needs to be
brought to the attention of the relevant authorities.
|
| References |
- Gray N. Report of the Chairman of the Education Committee.
Australian Cancer Society Annual Report, 1979. Sydney: ACS, 1979: 9.
-
Armstrong BK. Stratospheric ozone and health. Int J
Epidemiol 1994; 23: 873-885.
-
Arthey S, Clarke V. Suntanning and Sun protection: A review of the
psychological literature. Soc Sci Med 1995; 40 (2): 265-274.
-
Clarke V, Williams T, Arthey S. Skin type and optimistic bias in
relation to the sun protection and suntanning behaviors of young
adults. J Behav Med 1997; 20: 207-222.
-
Chapman S. Faking it: should cancer control agencies promote fake
tanning lotions? Med J Aust 1999; 170: 603-604.
-
Broadstock M, Borland R, Hill D. Knowledge, attitudes and reported
behaviours relevant to sun protection and suntanning in
adolescents. Psychol Health 1996; 11: 527-539.
-
Purchase M, Borland R. Public reaction to the 1992/93 SunSmart
campaign: results from a representative survey of Victorians.
SunSmart Evaluation Studies 3. Melbourne: Anti-Cancer Council of
Victoria, 1994: 93.
-
Dixon H, Cappiello M, Borland R. Reaction to the 1994/95 SunSmart
campaign: results from a representative household survey of
Victorians. SunSmart Evaluation Studies 5. Melbourne: Anti-Cancer
Council of Victoria, 1997: 64.
-
Stata version 6 [computer program]. College Station, TX: Stata
Corporation, 1999.
-
Huber PJ. The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates under
non-standard conditions. Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley
Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability 1967; 1:
221-233.
-
White H. A heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix
estimator and direct test for heteroskedasticity.
Econometrica 1980; 48: 817-830.
(Received 21 Mar, accepted 31 Jul, 2000)
|
Authors' Details | |
Anti-Cancer Foundation of South Australia, Adelaide, SA.
Kerri R Beckmann, BSc(Hons), MPH, Program Evaluation
Officer; Barbara A Kirke, DipN, MPHC, Skin Cancer Prevention
Project Officer.
Collaborative Research Centre for Asthma, University Department of
Medicine, Sir Charles Gairdner Hospital, Perth, WA.
Kieran A McCaul, BSc, MPH, Biostatistician.
Epidemiology Branch, South Australian Department of Human
Services, Adelaide, SA.
David M Roder, AM, MPH, DDSc, Director.
Reprints will not be available from the authors. Correspondence: Ms K
R Beckmann, Anti-Cancer Foundation of South Australia, PO Box 929,
Unley, SA 5061.
kbeckmannATcancersa.org.au
Make a
comment
|
| | | | | | Back to text | | | |
| | Back to text | | | |
| | Back to text | | | |
4: Factors associated with being burnt
more than once over summer |
|
All respondents |
Women only |
|
|
|
|
Adjusted |
|
|
Adjusted |
|
|
|
odds ratio |
95% CI |
P |
odds ratio |
95% CI |
P |
|
Fake
tan use |
Non-users
Users |
1.00
2.07 |
-
1.17-3.69 |
0.013 |
2.47 |
1.38-4.42 |
0.002 |
Sex |
Female
Male |
1.00
2.99 |
-
2.09-4.29 |
|
- |
- |
- |
Age
(group years) |
65+
18-24
25-34
35-44
45-54
55-64 |
1.00
13.70
5.83
4.72
2.46
1.27 |
-
7.00-26.78
3.19-10.65
2.66-8.38
1.34-4.53
0.61-2.66 |
0.004
0.529 |
15.46
4.40
5.12
3.56
0.40 |
5.34-44.77
1.71-11.34
2.04-12.86
1.37-9.23
0.09-1.65 |
0.002
0.001
0.009
0.203 |
Skin
type |
Just tan
Burn then tan
Just burn |
1.00
2.07
2.47 |
-
1.20-3.56
1.47-4.16 |
0.009
0.001 |
1.86
2.12 |
0.69-5.01
0.83-5.39 |
0.222
0.117 |
Sunscreen
use |
Irregular
Regular |
1.00
1.03 |
-
0.72-1.48 |
0.876 |
0.82 |
0.44-1.49 |
0.509 |
Hat
wearing |
Irregular
Regular |
1.00
0.69 |
0.48-0.98 |
0.038 |
0.91 |
0.52-1.59 |
0.746 |
Protective
clothing |
Irregular
Regular |
1.00
0.63 |
-
| 0.44-0.91
|
0.014 |
0.95 |
0.56-1.61 |
0.856 |
Shade
seeking |
Irregular
Regular |
1.00
0.91 |
-
0.62-1.32 |
0.604 |
0.55 |
0.30-1.01 |
0.056 |
|
*Logistic regression modelling
using forced entry of all variables. Separate models for all respondents
and women only. |
|
| | Back to text |
|